- [Highly recommended] practice quizzes from our textbook publisher. You will need to create an account (a very quick process).
- A quiz specifically on Research Methods.
- On this site, look under "Research Methods" to find three different quizzes for the unit.
Friday, September 19, 2014
Some Practice Quizzes for Unit 2
Tuesday, September 16, 2014
Article Clarifying "Correlation vs. Causation"
Correlation
and causation: what does it all mean?
Psychology Today, 2010
Adi Jaffe, Ph D.
Being
clear about inferences in research
Correlation
When
researchers say they have found a “correlation," what they are saying is that they found a relationship between two or
more variables. For instance, researchers have found a correlation between
using marijuana as a teen and having more troublesome relationships in mid- to
late-twenties.
Correlations
can be positive, meaning that as one variable (marijuana smoking) goes up, so does
the other (relationship trouble); or they can be negative, which would mean
that as one variable goes up (methamphetamine smoking)
another goes down (grade point average). The trouble is that unless they are properly
controlled for, there could be other variables affecting this relationship that
the researchers don't know about. For
instance: education, gender, and mental health
issues could be behind the marijuana-relationship association. (These variables were all controlled for by
the researchers in that study.)
Researchers
have at their disposal a number of sophisticated statistical tools to control
for these, ranging from the relatively simple (like multiple
regression) to the highly complex and involved (multi-level
modeling and structural
equation modeling). These methods
allow researchers to separate the effect of one variable from others, thereby
leaving them more confident in making assertions about the true nature of
the relationships they found. Still,
even under the best analysis circumstances, correlation is not the same as
causation.
Causation
When
an article says that causation was found, this means that the researchers found
that changes in one variable they measured directly caused changes
in the other. An example would be
research showing that jumping off a cliff directly causes great physical
damage. In order to do this, researchers
would need to assign people to jump off a
cliff and measure the amount of physical damage caused. When they find that jumping off the cliff
causes more damage, they can assert causality. Good luck recruiting for that study!
Most
of the research you read about indicates a correlation—NOT causation—between
variables. You can find the key words by
carefully reading. If the article says
something like "men were found to have," or "women were more
likely to," they're talking about correlation, not causation.
Why the difference?
The
reason is that in order to actually be able to claim causation, the researchers
have to split the participants into different groups and assign them the
behavior they want to study (like taking a new drug), while the rest don't. This is in fact what happens in clinical
trials of medication because the FDA
requires proof that the medication actually makes people better
(more so than a placebo). It's this random assignment to conditions that makes
experiments suitable for the discovery of causality. Unlike in correlational studies, random
assignment assures (if everything is designed correctly) that it’s the behavior
being studied, and not some other random effect, that is causing the
outcome.
Obviously,
it is much more difficult to prove causation than it is to prove an
association.
So, should we just ignore correlation?
No!
Not at all! Correlations are
crucial for research and still need to be looked at and studied, especially in
some areas of research like addiction.
The
reason is simple: we can't randomly
give people drugs like methamphetamine as children and study their brain development to
see how the stuff affects them, because that would be unethical. So what
we're left with is the study of what meth use (and use of other drugs) correlates
with. It's for this reason that
researchers use special statistical methods to assess correlations, making
certain that they are also considering other things that may be interfering
with their results.
In
the case of the marijuana article, the researchers ruled out a number of other
interfering variables known to affect relationships, like aggression, gender,
education, closeness with other family members, etc. By doing so, they did their best to assure
that the association found between marijuana and relationship status was real. Obviously other possibilities exist, but as
more researchers assess this relationship in different ways, we'll learn more
about its true nature.
This
is how research works.
It's
also how we found out that smoking causes cancer: through endlessly repeated findings showing
an association. That turned out pretty well, I think ...
Friday, September 12, 2014
Sunday, September 7, 2014
Textbook Website
Here is a link to the companion website for our textbook. By doing a quick registration (name and email address), you will be able to access practice quizzes for each unit.
Friday, September 5, 2014
Practice Quizzes for Unit 1
Recommended practice quizzes to use in securing/checking retention of reading content and preparing for the Unit 1 exam:
- Learnerator Guide for AP Psychology (three separate quizzes for different topics within Unit 1; look under "History and Approaches")
- "Schools of Psychology" Quiz (all Unit 1)
- Thinkspot Chapter 1 Quiz (all Unit 1)
Wednesday, September 3, 2014
Notetaking & Study Recommendations
Per our discussion of research on note-taking, here are some (hopefully helpful) links and images.
1. An article about Henry Roediger's research: "Science of learning book offers tips to ‘Make it Stick'"
2. The full interview with Henry Roediger
3. Cornell Notes guide:
1. An article about Henry Roediger's research: "Science of learning book offers tips to ‘Make it Stick'"
2. The full interview with Henry Roediger
3. Cornell Notes guide:
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)